Over the three-day blood drive, although the baseline donation rate from the previous year was about 0.2 percent per day, students in the obligatory condition were more than twice as likely to donate blood than those in the voluntary condition, at roughly 0.4 percent versus 0.2 percent. That the donation rate of the voluntary group matched that of the generic blood drive the previous year suggests making publicity mandatory really did affect donation behavior, Yang and Hsee say. They argue that obligatory publicity removes people’s fear of coming off as self-promotional because it places the decision outside a person’s control. “We suggest that obligatory publicity resolves the tension between pure and impure motives for charitable acts,” they write, “and offers a more effective strategy to promote charitable acts in public.”
In the same experiment, the researchers find that not only were participants in the obligatory group more likely to donate blood, but so were their friends who were not part of the experiment, suggesting obligatory publicity may generate a ripple effect through social networks. This could be because donors in the obligatory-publicity campaign were more comfortable displaying and discussing their stamp, which led them to recruit more friends, Yang and Hsee argue.
Other experiments using different charitable acts or hypothetical amounts of money also demonstrate that obligatory publicity tends to be more effective than the voluntary form.
However, the effect may not work if you think your peers don’t care about your giving behavior—or if the amount involved is on the low side, according to two final experiments. In one, telling participants that hypothetical peers weren’t concerned about tokens of charitable behavior diminished the effect. In the other, setting the bar at just $5 turned people off, compared with requiring publicity for $48 donations. The findings suggest that people are indeed sensitive to others’ perceptions, and they don’t expect a “cheap donation” to earn them a good reputation.
“The implications of this research are not limited to the marketing of charity campaigns,” the researchers write. “The conflict between genuine prosocial intentions and social image motives permeates numerous social phenomena, including interpersonal helping, cooperation, organizational citizenship, social enterprise, and cause-related marketing.”
People whose job is to drive selfless behavior in any form may want to keep the obligatory-publicity effect in mind. Whether encouraging large charitable donations or just good citizenship at work, organizations may be wise to take the onus off the individual and simply require publicizing good behavior.